Autor: admin
Datum objave: 29.08.2019
Share


Many Genes Influence Same-Sex Sexuality, Not a Single ‘Gay Gene’

The largest study of same-sex sexual behavior finds the genetics are complicated, and social and environmental factors are also key.

Many Genes Influence Same-Sex Sexuality, Not a Single ‘Gay Gene’

https://www.nytimes.com/2019/08/29/science/gay-gene-sex.html


The largest study of same-sex sexual behavior finds the genetics are complicated, and social and environmental factors are also key.


By Pam Belluck


Benjamin Neale, a geneticist at the Broad Institute at M.I.T. and Harvard University and a lead researcher in the study.CreditCreditKayana Szymczak for The New York Times

An ambitious new study — the largest ever to analyze the genetics of same-sex sexual behavior — found that genetics does play a role, responsible for perhaps a third of the influence on whether someone has same-sex sex. The influence comes not from one gene but many, each with a tiny effect — and the rest of the explanation includes social or environmental factors — making it impossible to use genes to predict someone’s sexuality.

Sign up for Science Times

We’ll bring you stories that capture the wonders of the human body, nature and the cosmos.

SIGN UP


Large-scale GWAS reveals insights into the genetic architecture of same-sex sexual behavior

https://science.sciencemag.org/content/365/6456/eaat7693

The genetics of sexual orientation

Twin studies and other analyses of inheritance of sexual orientation in humans has indicated that same-sex sexual behavior has a genetic component. Previous searches for the specific genes involved have been underpowered and thus unable to detect genetic signals. Ganna et al. perform a genome-wide association study on 493,001 participants from the United States, the United Kingdom, and Sweden to study genes associated with sexual orientation (see the Perspective by Mills). They find multiple loci implicated in same-sex sexual behavior indicating that, like other behavioral traits, nonheterosexual behavior is polygenic.

Science, this issue p. eaat7693; see also p. 869

Structured Abstract

INTRODUCTION

Across human societies and in both sexes, some 2 to 10% of individuals report engaging in sex with same-sex partners, either exclusively or in addition to sex with opposite-sex partners. Twin and family studies have shown that same-sex sexual behavior is partly genetically influenced, but previous searches for the specific genes involved have been underpowered to detect effect sizes realistic for complex traits.

RATIONALE

For the first time, new large-scale datasets afford sufficient statistical power to identify genetic variants associated with same-sex sexual behavior (ever versus never had a same-sex partner), estimate the proportion of variation in the trait accounted for by all variants in aggregate, estimate the genetic correlation of same-sex sexual behavior with other traits, and probe the biology and complexity of the trait. To these ends, we performed genome-wide association discovery analyses on 477,522 individuals from the United Kingdom and United States, replication analyses in 15,142 individuals from the United States and Sweden, and follow-up analyses using different aspects of sexual preference.

RESULTS

In the discovery samples (UK Biobank and 23andMe), five autosomal loci were significantly associated with same-sex sexual behavior. Follow-up of these loci suggested links to biological pathways that involve sex hormone regulation and olfaction. Three of the loci were significant in a meta-analysis of smaller, independent replication samples. Although only a few loci passed the stringent statistical corrections for genome-wide multiple testing and were replicated in other samples, our analyses show that many loci underlie same-sex sexual behavior in both sexes. In aggregate, all tested genetic variants accounted for 8 to 25% of variation in male and female same-sex sexual behavior, and the genetic influences were positively but imperfectly correlated between the sexes [genetic correlation coefficient (rg)= 0.63; 95% confidence intervals, 0.48 to 0.78]. These aggregate genetic influences partly overlapped with those on a variety of other traits, including externalizing behaviors such as smoking, cannabis use, risk-taking, and the personality trait “openness to experience.” Additional analyses suggested that sexual behavior, attraction, identity, and fantasies are influenced by a similar set of genetic variants (rg > 0.83); however, the genetic effects that differentiate heterosexual from same-sex sexual behavior are not the same as those that differ among nonheterosexuals with lower versus higher proportions of same-sex partners, which suggests that there is no single continuum from opposite-sex to same-sex preference.

CONCLUSION

Same-sex sexual behavior is influenced by not one or a few genes but many. Overlap with genetic influences on other traits provides insights into the underlying biology of same-sex sexual behavior, and analysis of different aspects of sexual preference underscore its complexity and call into question the validity of bipolar continuum measures such as the Kinsey scale. Nevertheless, many uncertainties remain to be explored, including how sociocultural influences on sexual preference might interact with genetic influences. To help communicate our study to the broader public, we organized workshops in which representatives of the public, activists, and researchers discussed the rationale, results, and implications of our study.

Download high-res image

 Open in new tab

 Download Powerpoint

A genome-wide association study (GWAS) of same-sex sexual behavior reveals five loci and high polygenicity.

Follow-up analyses show potential biological pathways; show genetic correlations with various traits; and indicate that sexual preference is a complex, heterogeneous phenotype.

Abstract

Twin and family studies have shown that same-sex sexual behavior is partly genetically influenced, but previous searches for specific genes involved have been underpowered. We performed a genome-wide association study (GWAS) on 477,522 individuals, revealing five loci significantly associated with same-sex sexual behavior. In aggregate, all tested genetic variants accounted for 8 to 25% of variation in same-sex sexual behavior, only partially overlapped between males and females, and do not allow meaningful prediction of an individual’s sexual behavior. Comparing these GWAS results with those for the proportion of same-sex to total number of sexual partners among nonheterosexuals suggests that there is no single continuum from opposite-sex to same-sex sexual behavior. Overall, our findings provide insights into the genetics underlying same-sex sexual behavior and underscore the complexity of sexuality.

Across human societies and in both sexes, some 2 to 10% of individuals report engaging in sex with same-sex partners, either exclusively or in addition to sex with opposite-sex partners (1–4). The biological factors that contribute to sexual preference are largely unknown (5), but genetic influences are suggested by the observation that same-sex sexual behavior appears to run in families (6) and is concordant more often in genetically identical (monozygotic) twin pairs than in fraternal twin pairs or siblings (7). With respect to genetic influences, several questions arise. First, what genes are involved and what biological processes do they affect? Previous reports of genetic variants associated with sexual orientation (8–10) were based on relatively small samples and did not meet current standards of genome-wide significance (P < 5 × 10−8). Identification of robustly associated variants could enable exploration of the biological pathways and processes involved in development of same-sex sexual behavior. One hypothesis suggests that sex hormones are involved (11–13), but little direct genetic or biological evidence is available. Second, to what extent are genetic influences the same or different for females and males; behavior, attraction, and identity; and heterosexuality and different same-sex sexual behaviors (such as bisexuality)?

In order to identify genetic variants associated with same-sex sexual behavior and explore its genetic architecture and underlying biology, we performed a genome-wide association study (GWAS) of same-sex sexual behavior. Analyses were conducted in the UK Biobank from the United Kingdom and a cohort of research participants from 23andMe, predominantly located in the United States, and replications were performed in three other smaller studies. This study is part of a preregistered research plan (Open Science Framework; https://osf.io/357tn), and we explain our deviations from that plan in (14).

Phenotypic characterization

The UK Biobank study comprises a sample of ~500,000 genotyped UK residents aged 40 to 70 years (tables S1 and S2) (14). Our primary phenotype of interest is a binary, self-reported measure of whether respondents had ever had sex with someone of the same sex (here termed “nonheterosexuals”) or had not (here termed “heterosexuals”) (Box 1).

Box 1

Phenotype and sample definition and limitations.

In this study, we use the term “same-sex sexual behavior,” which is defined as having ever had sex with someone of the same sex. Detailed descriptions of the variables used in the different cohorts can be found in the supplementary materials (14).

To aid in readability throughout the manuscript, in some places we refer to individuals who have ever had sex with someone of the same sex as “nonheterosexuals,” whereas we refer to individuals who have never had sex with someone of the same sex as “heterosexuals.”

We acknowledge that the grouping phrase “nonheterosexuals” has the potential to present messages of othering (that is, undesirable marginalization of another person or group on the basis of their sexual expression)—by defining an “outgroup” in reference to an “ingroup” and implying that “nonheterosexual behavior” may have a negative connotation, whereas “heterosexual behavior” may have a positive one. We wish to make clear that our choice of language is not meant to forward messages of othering on the basis of sexual behavior.

Throughout this manuscript, we use the terms “female” and “male” rather than “woman” and “man.” This is because our analyses and results relate to biologically defined sex, not to gender.

As is common in genetic analyses, we dropped individuals from our study whose biological sex and self-identified sex/gender did not match. This is an important limitation of our analyses because the analyses do not include transgender persons, intersex persons, and other important persons and groups within the queer community. We hope that this limitation will be addressed in future work.

In the UK Biobank sample, 4.1% of males and 2.8% of females reported ever having had sex with someone of the same sex (tables S1 and S2), with higher rates among younger participants (Fig. 1A). This binary phenotype follows from previous work proposing that sexual preference is taxonic rather than dimensional in structure, with individuals reporting exclusively opposite-sex preference differing from individuals reporting any same-sex preference (15). However, the binary variable also collapses rich and multifaceted diversity among nonheterosexual individuals (15), so we explored finer-scaled measurements and some of the complexities of the phenotype, although intricacies of the social and cultural influences on sexuality made it impossible to fully explore this complexity. The 23andMe sample comprised 23andMe customers who consented to participate in research and chose to complete a survey about sexual orientation (from many possible survey topics). Individuals who engage in same-sex sexual behavior may be more likely to self-select the sexual orientation survey, which would explain the unusually high proportion of individuals who had had same-sex sexual partners in this sample (18.9%) (table S3) (14).

Fig. 1 Descriptive statistics regarding same-sex sexual behavior in the UK Biobank.

The percentage of participants in the UK Biobank who reported having had at least one same-sex sexual partner (y axis) increased with participants’ year of birth (x axis). (B) Among participants reporting at least one same-sex partner, those with a greater proportion of same-sex partners (x axis) have a larger reproductive disadvantage (lower birth-year adjusted number of children) (y axis). Vertical bars represent 95% CIs.

We also performed replication analyses in three smaller datasets (14): (i) Molecular Genetic Study of Sexual Orientation (MGSOSO) (n = 2308 U.S. adult males), in which respondents were asked about their sexual identity; (ii) Add Health (n = 4755 U.S. young adults), in which respondents were asked whether they ever had same-sex intercourse and whether they were romantically attracted to the same sex; and (iii) Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS) (n = 8093 Swedish adolescents), in which participants reported the degree of attraction to the same versus opposite sex.

We observed in the UK Biobank that individuals who reported same-sex sexual behavior had on average fewer offspring than those of individuals who engaged exclusively in heterosexual behavior, even for individuals reporting only a minority of same-sex partners (Fig. 1B). This reduction in number of children is comparable with or greater than for other traits that have been linked to lower fertility rates (fig. S1) (14). This reproductive deficit raises questions about the evolutionary maintenance of the trait, but we do not address these here.

Genetic architecture of same-sex sexual behavior

We first assessed whether same-sex sexual behavior clustered in families in a manner consistent with genetic influences on the phenotype. Among pairs of individuals in the UK Biobank related at full cousin or closer [as identified by genomic similarity (14); n pairs = 106,979], more closely related individuals were more likely to be concordant in terms of same-sex sexual behavior. By modeling the correspondence of relatedness among individuals and the similarity of their sexual behavior, we estimated broad-sense heritability—the percentage of variation in a trait attributable to genetic variation—at 32.4% [95% confidence intervals (CIs), 10.6 to 54.3] (table S4). This

estimate is consistent with previous estimates from smaller twin studies (7).

To identify genetic variants [largely single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs)] associated with same-sex sexual behavior, we performed a GWAS in the UK Biobank study (n = 408,995 individuals) (14). To increase power and generalizability of our results, we also performed a GWAS in the cohort from 23andMe using an equivalent variable (individuals who reported having had sex with “Other sex only” versus the other options on a seven-point scale regarding participants’ sexual partners) (n = 68,527 individuals, of which 12,933 reported same-sex sexual behavior) (table S3) (14). We estimated the genetic correlation (16) between different heritable traits to determine the degree of consistency of genetic influences on same-sex sexual behavior in the two studies, which was high [genetic correlation coefficient (rg) = 0.87; 95% CIs, 0.67 to 1.06] (table S5) (14). Genetic correlations between same-sex sexual behavior and 28 different traits were largely similar in the UK Biobank and 23andMe (fig. S2) (14), although a few differences were observed; for example, in females, the genetic correlations between same-sex sexual behavior and anorexia were in opposite directions in the UK Biobank (rg = –0.36; 95% CIs, –0.60 and –0.09) and 23andMe data (rg = 0.36; 95% CIs, 0.08 to 0.65; Wald test P value for differences = 0.0001). Overall, these results indicate that the genetic influences on same-sex sexual behavior in the two samples is similar, although there is some suggestion of phenotypic heterogeneity. We meta-analyzed the two sample sets using MTAG (17), which models their genetic correlation to determine the meta-analytic weights, yielding a total sample size of 477,522 individuals (26,827 individuals reporting same-sex sexual behavior).

After standard quality control checks (table S6) (14), we identified two genome-wide significant signals for same-sex sexual behavior (rs11114975-12q21.31 and rs10261857-7q31.2) (Fig. 2 and tables S7 and S8). We discuss these SNPs further in the section “In-silico follow-up of GWAS results.” To assess differences in effects between females and males, we also performed sex-specific analyses. These results suggested only a partially shared genetic architecture across the sexes; the across-sex genetic correlation was 0.63 (95% CIs, 0.48 to 0.78) (table S9). This is noteworthy given that most other studied traits show much higher across-sex genetic correlations, often close to 1 (18–21). Through the sex-specific analyses, we identified two additional signals in males (rs28371400-15q21.3 and rs34730029-11q12.1), which showed no significant association in females, and one in females (rs13135637-4p14), which showed no significant association in

males. Overall, three of the SNPs replicated at a nominal P value in the meta-analyzed replication datasets (Wald test P = 0.027 for rs34730029, P = 0.003 for rs28371400, and P = 0.006 for rs11114975) (table S10), despite the much smaller sample size (MGSOSO, Add Health, and CATSS; total sample size = 15,156 individuals, effective sample size = 4887 individuals).

Download high-res image

Download Powerpoint

Fig. 2 Manhattan plot for a GWAS of same-sex sexual behavior.

Diamonds (red) represent genome-wide significant signals from analysis of males and females combined, and triangles represent genome-wide significant signals that are female (pointing up, blue) or male (pointing down, green) specific.

The SNPs that reached genome-wide significance had very small effects (odds ratios ~1.1) (table S7). For example, in the UK Biobank, males with a GT genotype at the rs34730029 locus had 0.4% higher prevalence of same-sex sexual behavior than those with a TT genotype (4.0 versus 3.6%). Nevertheless, the contribution of all measured common SNPs in aggregate (SNP-based heritability) was estimated to be 8 to 25% (95% CIs, 5 to 30%) of variation in female and male same-sex sexual behavior, in which the range reflects differing estimates by using different analysis methods or prevalence assumptions (table S11) (14). The discrepancy between the variance captured by the significant SNPs and all common SNPs suggests that same-sex sexual behavior, like most complex human traits, is influenced by the small, additive effects of very many genetic variants, most of which cannot be detected at the current sample size (22). Consistent with this interpretation, we show that the contribution of each chromosome to heritability is broadly proportional to its size (fig. S3) (14). In contrast to linkage studies that found substantial association of sexual orientation with variants on the X-chromosome (8, 23), we found no excessof signal (and no individual genome-wide significant loci) on the X-chromosome (fig. S4).

To test whether these aggregate estimates of genetic effects correlate with sexuality in other samples, we constructed polygenic scores for same-sex sexual behavior (14, 24). These polygenic scores were significantly associated with sexual identity in MGSOSO (Wald test, P = 0.001) and same-sex attraction in the Add Health (P = 0.017) and CATSS (P = 3.5 × 10−6) studies (tables S12, S13, and S14). In CATSS, polygenic scores were also significantly associated with sexual attraction in participants at age 15 years (P = 6.4 × 10−5), suggesting that at least some of the genetic influences on same-sex sexual behavior manifest early in sexual development. The purpose of these analyses is to further characterize the genetic influences on same-sex sexual behavior and not to predict same-sex sexual behavior on the individual level. In all cases, the variance explained by the polygenic scores was extremely low (<1%); these scores could not be used to accurately predict sexual behavior in an individual.

Overall, these findings suggest that genetic influences on same-sex sexual behavior are highly polygenic and are not specific to the discovery samples or measures. All the SNPs measured, when combined, do not capture the entirety of family-based heritability (8 to 25% from GWAS versus 32% from family-based methods). In this, same-sex sexual behavior is similar to many other complex traits; the ratio between family-based heritability and SNP-heritability estimated in the same sample is consistent with empirical findings for the other 16 traits we tested (family heritability approximately three times larger than SNP-heritability) (Fig. 3) (14). There are many possible reasons for this discrepancy, including, but not limited to, variants not captured by genotyping arrays, nonadditive genetic effects, and phenotypic heterogeneity.

of signal (and no individual genome-wide significant loci) on the X-chromosome (fig. S4).

To test whether these aggregate estimates of genetic effects correlate with sexuality in other samples, we constructed polygenic scores for same-sex sexual behavior (14, 24). These polygenic scores were significantly associated with sexual identity in MGSOSO (Wald test, P = 0.001) and same-sex attraction in the Add Health (P = 0.017) and CATSS (P = 3.5 × 10−6) studies (tables S12, S13, and S14). In CATSS, polygenic scores were also significantly associated with sexual attraction in participants at age 15 years (P = 6.4 × 10−5), suggesting that at least some of the genetic influences on same-sex sexual behavior manifest early in sexual development. The purpose of these analyses is to further characterize the genetic influences on same-sex sexual behavior and not to predict same-sex sexual behavior on the individual level. In all cases, the variance explained by the polygenic scores was extremely low (<1%); these scores could not be used to accurately predict sexual behavior in an individual.

Overall, these findings suggest that genetic influences on same-sex sexual behavior are highly polygenic and are not specific to the discovery samples or measures. All the SNPs measured, when combined, do not capture the entirety of family-based heritability (8 to 25% from GWAS versus 32% from family-based methods). In this, same-sex sexual behavior is similar to many other complex traits; the ratio between family-based heritability and SNP-heritability estimated in the same sample is consistent with empirical findings for the other 16 traits we tested (family heritability approximately three times larger than SNP-heritability) (Fig. 3) (14). There are many possible reasons for this discrepancy, including, but not limited to, variants not captured by genotyping arrays, nonadditive genetic effects, and phenotypic heterogeneity.

At the level of individual loci, we investigated biological pathways by integrating information from expression quantitative trait loci (eQTL) analyses (27), phenome-wide association study (PheWAS) (table S16) (28), and gene-based analysis by using MAGMA (14, 29). A full report can be found in table S17. Here, we highlight findings relating to the two SNPs associated with male same-sex sexual behavior: rs34730029 and rs28371400. First, the locus encompassing rs34730029-11q12.1 contains a number of olfactory receptor genes (several of which were significantly associated with same-sex sexual behavior in a gene-based test) (fig. S6 and table S18). This SNP is correlated [linkage disequilibrium, coefficient of determination (R2) = 0.70] with a missense variant (rs6591536) in OR5A1 that has been reported to have a substantial effect on the sensitivity to certain scents (30). Second, rs28371400-15q21.3 had several indications of being involved in sex hormone regulation: The allele positively associated with same-sex sexual behavior is associated with higher rate of male pattern balding [in which sex-hormone sensitivity is implicated (31)] and is located ~20 kb upstream of the TCF12 gene. TCF12 is the primary heterodimerization partner for TCF21, a transcription factor essential for normal development of the gonads in mice (32), and is involved in the downstream actions of the SRY gene (which is responsible for the initiation of male sex determination) in humans (33).

Genetic correlations with other traits

Next, we explored the genetic correlations between same-sex sexual behavior and 28 other relevant traits chosen before the analyses, using summary statistics from other GWASs (Fig. 4 and table S19) (14). In particular, we included mental health traits because they are substantially heritable (34), and previous population surveys have shown elevated risk of adverse mental health outcomes (such as depression, anxiety, or substance use) in sexual minority populations, including individuals engaging in same-sex sexual behavior (35, 36).

Fig. 4 Genetic correlations of same-sex sexual behavior with various preselected traits and disorders, separately for males and females.

Males, green; females, blue. Yellow asterisks denote the genetic correlations that were experiment-wise significant (P < 8.9 × 10−4; references, definitions, and full results can be found in table S19). Wald test P values for the genetic correlations are reported above each dot. Horizontal bars represent 95% CIs.

We found several personality traits (loneliness and openness to experience), risky behaviors (smoking and cannabis use) and mental health disorders, but not physical traits, to be significantly genetically correlated with same-sex sexual behavior. We found in both sexes that same-sex sexual behavior was positively genetically correlated with several psychiatric or mental health traits [for example, depression, rg = 0.44 in females (95% CIs, 0.32 and 0.55), rg = 0.33 in males (95% CIs, 0.22 and 0.43); schizophrenia, rg = 0.17 in females (95% CIs 0.08 and 0.35), rg = 0.13 in males (95% CIs, 0.05 and 0.26); all Wald test P < 0.001]. We emphasize that the causal processes underlying these genetic correlations are unclear and could be generated by environmental factors relating to prejudice against individuals engaging in same-sex sexual behavior, among other possibilities, which we discuss in (14). Some associations were sex specific. In particular, the genetic correlations with bipolar disorder, cannabis use, and number of sexual partners were significantly higher in females than in males (Wald test P = 0.001, 1.47 × 10−6, and 3.13 × 10−5 respectively) (table S19).

Last, given the potential roles of sex hormones in sexual behaviors, we directly explored whether there is a genetic correlation with serum sex-hormone–binding globulin (SHBG) levels (37), which are thought to be inversely related to bioactive testosterone and estrogen in females and males, respectively (38). There was a significant correlation in females (rg = 0.25, Wald test P = 0.03) but not in males (rg = 0.10, Wald test P = 0.32).

Complexity and heterogeneity

To maximize our sample size and increase the power to detect SNP associations, we defined our primary phenotype as ever or never having had a same sex partner. Such a measure fails to capture the multifaceted richness and complexity of human sexual orientation. To explore the consequences of this simplification, we pursued genetic analyses across different aspects of sexual preference and behavior.

First, within participants reporting same-sex sexual behavior, we performed a GWAS on the proportion of same-sex partners to total partners, with a higher value indicating a higher proportion of same-sex partners (14). In the UK Biobank, this is measured directly from participants’ reported number of same-sex and all partners, whereas in 23andMe, we used participants’ raw responses to the item “With whom have you had sex?”, which in individuals reporting same-sex sexual behavior could be “other sex mostly,” “other sex slightly,” “equal,” “same sex slightly,” “same sex mostly,” or “same sex only.” The UK Biobank and 23andMe variables were heritable (table S20A) and genetically correlated with each other (rg = 0.52 and 95% CIs, –0.16 to 1.20 for females; rg = 0.73 and 95% CIs, 0.18 to 1.27 for males) (Fig. 5A and table S20C), so we used MTAG to meta-analyze across the two studies for subsequent analyses.

Fig. 5 Complexity and heterogeneity of genetic influences.

Genetic correlations between the main phenotype (same-sex sexual behavior; heterosexuals versus nonheterosexuals) and proportion of same-sex to total sexual partners among nonheterosexuals, in the UK Biobank and 23andMe samples. (B) Scatterplot showing genetic correlations of the main phenotype (x axis) and the proportion of same-sex to total partners among nonheterosexuals (y axis) with various other traits (table S21). (C) Genetic correlations among different sexual preference items in the 23andMe sample.

We found little evidence for genetic correlation of the proportion of same-sex to total partners among individuals reporting same-sex sexual behavior (nonheterosexuals) with the binary same-sex sexual behavior variable [rg = –0.31 (95% CIs, –0.62 to 0.00) for females and rg = 0.03 (95% CIs, –0.18 to 0.23) for males] (table S20B). Further, this phenotype showed a markedly different pattern of genetic correlations with other traits, as compared with corresponding genetic correlations with the binary same-sex sexual behavior variable (Fig. 5B and table S21). These findings suggest that the same-sex sexual behavior variable and the proportion of same-sex partners among nonheterosexuals capture aspects of sexuality that are distinct on the genetic level, which in turn suggests that there is no single continuum from opposite-sex to same-sex sexual behavior. Interpretations of any one set of results in our study must consider this complexity.

With this in mind, we examined the possibility of different genetic variants distinguishing heterosexual behavior from differing proportions of same-sex partners within nonheterosexuals. To do so, we performed additional GWASs in the UK Biobank data on the following traits: those whose partners were (i) less than a third same-sex, (ii) between a third and two-thirds same-sex, (iii) more than a third same-sex, and (iv) exclusively same-sex. Genetic correlations of the first three categories with the fourth were 0.13, 0.80, and 0.95 (table S22), indicating partly different genetic variants distinguishing heterosexual behavior from differing proportions of same-sex partners within nonheterosexuals.

Last, using additional measures from 23andMe, we showed strong genetic correlations (all rg 0.83) (Fig. 5C and fig. S7) of same-sex sexual behavior with items assessing same-sex attraction, identity, and fantasies (a full list of items is provided in table S5), suggesting that these different aspects of sexual orientation are influenced by largely the same genetic variants. The full set of results of phenotypic and genetic correlations for females, males, and the whole sample is available in fig. S7 and table S5.

Discussion

We identified genome-wide significant loci associated with same-sex sexual behavior and found evidence of a broader contribution of common genetic variation. We established that the underlying genetic architecture is highly complex; there is certainly no single genetic determinant (sometimes referred to as the “gay gene” in the media). Rather, many loci with individually small effects, spread across the whole genome and partly overlapping in females and males, additively contribute to individual differences in predisposition to same-sex sexual behavior. All measured common variants together explain only part of the genetic heritability at the population level and do not allow meaningful prediction of an individual’s sexual preference.

The knowledge that the variants involved are numerous and spread across the genome enabled us to leverage whole-genome analytic techniques to explore human sexual behavior in ways previously impossible. We determined that the genetic effects that differentiate heterosexual from same-sex sexual behavior are not the same as those that differ among nonheterosexuals with lower versus higher proportions of same-sex partners. This finding suggests that on the genetic level, there is no single dimension from opposite-sex to same-sex preference. The existence of such a dimension, in which the more someone is attracted to the same-sex the less they are attracted to the opposite-sex, is the premise of the Kinsey scale (39), a research tool ubiquitously used to measure sexual orientation. Another measure, the Klein Grid (40), retains the same premise but separately measures sexual attraction, behavior, fantasies, and identification (as well as nonsexual preferences); however, we found that these sexual measures are influenced by similar genetic factors. Overall, our findings suggest that the most popular measures are based on a misconception of the underlying structure of sexual orientation and may need to be rethought. In particular, using separate measures of attraction to the opposite sex and attraction to the same sex, such as in the Sell Assessment of Sexual Orientation (41), would remove the assumption that these variables are perfectly inversely related and would enable more nuanced exploration of the full diversity of sexual orientation, including bisexuality and asexuality.

Although we emphasize the polygenicity of the genetic effects on same-sex sexual behavior, we identified five SNPs whose association with same-sex sexual behavior reached genome-wide significance. Three of these replicated in other independent samples whose measures related to identity and attraction rather than behavior. These SNPs may serve to generate new lines of enquiry. In particular, the finding that one of the replicated SNPs (rs28371400-15q21.3) is linked to male pattern balding and is nearby a gene (TCF12) relevant to sexual differentiation strengthens the idea that sex-hormone regulation may be involved in the development of same-sex sexual behavior. Also, that another replicated SNP (rs34730029-11q12.1) is strongly linked to several genes involved in olfaction raises intriguing questions. Although the underlying mechanism at this locus is unclear, a link between olfaction and reproductive function has previously been established. Individuals with Kallmann syndrome exhibit both delayed or absent pubertal development and an impaired sense of smell because of the close developmental origin of fetal gonadotropin-releasing hormone and olfactory neurons (42).

Our study focused on the genetic basis of same-sex sexual behavior, but several of our results point to the importance of sociocultural context as well. We observed changes in prevalence of reported same-sex sexual behavior across time, raising questions about how genetic and sociocultural influences on sexual behavior might interact. We also observed partly different genetic influences on same-sex sexual behavior in females and males; this could reflect sex differences in hormonal influences on sexual behavior (for example, importance of testosterone versus estrogen) but could also relate to different sociocultural contexts of female and male same-sex behavior and different demographics of gay, lesbian, and bisexual groups (43). With these points in mind, we acknowledge the limitation that we only studied participants of European ancestry and from a few Western countries; research involving larger and more diverse samples will afford greater insight into how these findings fare across different sociocultural contexts.

Our findings provide insights into the biological underpinnings of same-sex sexual behavior but also underscore the importance of resisting simplistic conclusions (Box 2)—because the behavioral phenotypes are complex, because our genetic insights are rudimentary, and because there is a long history of misusing genetic results for social purposes.

Box 2

Communication and interpretation.

The topic explored in this study is complex and intersects with sexuality, identity, and attraction and potentially has civil and political implications for sexual minority groups. Therefore, we have

Engaged with science communication teams,

Engaged with LGBTQIA+ advocacy groups nationally and within our local institutions, and

Tried to make clear the many limitations and nuances of our study and our phenotypes.

We wish to make it clear that our results overwhelmingly point toward the richness and diversity of human sexuality. Our results do not point toward a role for discrimination on the basis of sexual identity or attraction, nor do our results make any conclusive statements about the degree to which “nature” and “nurture” influence sexual preference.

Materials and methods summary

Study samples

We used data from genotyped individuals from five cohorts (total n = 492,678) who provided self-report information using different questionnaire-based measurement scales. Informed consent was provided from all individuals participating in the studies, which were approved by their local research ethic committee.

Genetic association analyses

After standard quality control, we performed GWASs for “same-sex sexual behavior” (defined as ever versus never having had sex with a same-sex partner) in the UK Biobank and 23andMe samples, which we meta-analysed using MTAG (17). We also conducted GWASs separately by sex. Genome-wide significant SNPs were replicated in three independent samples. Also, using LD-pred (24), we derived polygenic score for same-sex sexual behavior according to the meta-analyzed GWAS results and tested the association between this polygenic score and same-sex sexual behavior in three independent samples. To explore diversity among individuals reporting same-sex sexual behavior, we also conducted GWASs in the UK-Biobank and 23andMe samples (meta-analyzed using MTAG) on the “proportion of same-sex to total number of sexual partners among nonheterosexuals.”

Heritability estimation

We estimated family-based heritability of same-sex sexual behavior on the basis of known familial relationships in the UK Biobank study. The relatedness between pairs of participants was estimated by using KING (44). Additive genetic effects as well as shared and unshared environmental variance components were estimated on the basis of the covariance between different pairs of relatives. Second, heritability explained by all measured common SNPs (SNP-based heritability) was estimated by using linkage disequilibrium (LD) score regression (45) and transformed to the liability scale (46). Using a similar approach, we also estimated the SNP-based heritability per chromosome and evaluated heritability enrichment across various tissues on the basis of Genotype-Tissue Expression (GTEx) gene-expression results (47).

In silico follow-up

The GWAS results for same-sex sexual behavior were followed up with gene-based tests of association in MAGMA (29) and an enrichment analysis of evolutionarily constrained genes by using partitioned LD score regression (45) and MAGMA. We also performed a PheWAS (28) to examine whether the SNPs we identified for same-sex sexual behavior have also been associated with other phenotypes and eQTL mapping (27) to link SNPs with gene expression.

Genetic correlations and phenotypic heterogeneity

Using cross-trait LD score regression (16), we estimated the genetic correlations of same-sex sexual behavior and proportion of same-sex to total number of sexual partners among nonheterosexuals with a range of traits, including mental health, personality, and sexually dimorphic traits. To examine heterogeneity of genetic influences, we looked at the genetic correlations between sexes, between cohorts, and between different measures of sexual preference.

Science communication strategy

To communicate the results of the study to the broader audience, we engaged with different LGBTQIA+ (lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer, intersex, asexual, and other+) and science communication organizations and created multimedia materials for a lay audience.

Detailed materials and methods can be found in the supplementary materials (14).

Supplementary Materials

science.sciencemag.org/content/365/6456/eaat7693 /suppl/DC1

Materials and Methods

Figs. S1 to S7

Tables S1 to S23

References (49–108)


http://www.sciencemag.org/about/science-licenses-journal-article-reuse

This is an article distributed under the terms of the Science Journals Default License.

References and Notes



BENJAMIN NEALE

https://www.broadinstitute.org/bios/benjamin-neale

Benjamin Neale

Institute Member

Benjamin Neale is director of genetics in the Stanley Center for Psychiatric Research at the Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard, where he is an institute member. He is an associate professor in the Analytic and Translational Genetics Unit (ATGU) at Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH), where he directs the Genomics of Public Health Initiative. He is also an associate professor in medicine at Harvard Medical School (HMS). Neale is strongly committed to gaining insights into the genetics of common, complex human diseases. Neale and Mark Daly, both of whom are associated with the Broad Institute and MGH, lead the ADHD Initiative, a collaborative effort that focuses on genomic studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Neale’s research and training have focused heavily on statistical methodology. He has analyzed genetic data from large-scale studies of patients with ADHD, autism, age-related macular degeneration, type 2 diabetes, and metabolic disorders. Neale also analyzed data from the first ADHD genome-wide association study (GWAS) meta-analysis, which combined the results of four studies to boost statistical power. Neale contributed to the development of software tools such as PLINK, one of the most frequently used packages for GWAS analysis. In addition to his roles at both the Broad Institute and MGH, Neale is the head of the ADHD psychiatric genetics GWAS analysis committee and an active member of the broader Psychiatric GWAS Consortium analysis committee, which is charged with analyzing all psychiatric data from these large-scale genome-wide association studies. Neale also led the design of the exome chip, a genotyping array that captures rare coding variation in a cost-effective manner. To date, over 1.5 million exome chips have been sold.

Neale studied at the University of Chicago and Virginia Commonwealth University, earning a B.Sc. in genetics. He went on to earn his Ph.D. in human genetics from King’s College in London, UK. Neale completed his postdoctoral training in Daly’s laboratory at Massachusetts General Hospital. In addition to many local research collaborations, he also serves as advisor and analyst to international genetic research consortia on psychiatric diseases.

August 2019



Broad View: Benjamin Neale - Progress in psychiatric genetics

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=UbNMb9mqQ2E



PEOPLE

http://www.nealelab.is/people




Pam Belluck

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Pam_Belluck

Pam Belluck, an American journalist and author, is a health and science writer for The New York Times and author of the acclaimed nonfiction book Island Practice, which is in development for a television series.

She was a member of The New York Times reporting team that received the 2015 Pulitzer Prize for International Reporting for coverage of the 2014 Ebola virus epidemic in West Africa. Team members named by The Times were Belluck, Helene Cooper, Sheri Fink, Adam Nossiter, Norimitsu Onishi, Kevin Sack, and Ben C. Solomon



505
Kategorije: Fenomeni
Developed by LELOO. All rights reserved.